Creating Stimuli For Web-base Self Paced Reading

Terminology

Design Types

Latin square design

The idea behind Latin square design (a between-subject design) is to have each participant see each item exactly once AND to see all conditions equally often AND across lists each item should be seen equally often in all its conditions. Since each participants sees exactly one list of the experiment, this means that each list should contain each item only once (in one of its condition) and all conditions equally often. One thing that follows from this is that the number of items that an experiment should have should be a multiple of the number of the experiment's conditions. For power consideration, we often have between 3- to 6-times as many items as there are conditions in the experiment (and an equal number of subjects, but that's another matter). For more subtle effects, you may need even more items.

How should the items and conditions be distributed across lists? Let's consider a 2 x 2 design, with the four conditions a1, b1, a2, and b2. The minimum number of lists we will need is four. Let's say that we use 8-times more items than condition (a.k.a. 8 items per condition) and that List1 looks like this (prior to sorting and prior to the inclusion of fillers):

So, List1 contains each item only once, and each conditions (a1, b1, a2, b2) occurs equally often (8 times). Now we want a second list that fulfills these constraints and brings us closer to the third constraints stated above, that --across lists-- each item should be seen equally often in all its conditions. To achieve this, we construct List2 by simply shifting the condition one up. So the condition of Item1 in List2 will be the one of Item2 in List1, etc. The last item of List2 will occur in the same condition as the first item of List1

If we repeat this for List3 and List4 (see below), you will see that --across lists-- each item occurs exactly once in each of its conditions, and --within lists-- each item occurs once and all conditions occur equally often across items. That's what we want. (Latin square designs are most powerful, if each item is seen equally often in each condition across all participants. That is, we want each list to be seen by equally many participants, but that is a matter to be kept track of later when we run the experiment)

etc.

NB: A convenient way to achieve this balancing in Excel is to follow the following steps:

  1. Have all your items in one sheet, row by row, and, for all item, conditions always are ordered in the same way. For example, if you have 32 items in 4 conditions (A, B, C, D), e.g. from a 2 x 2 design, then the rows would look like this:
    • Item 1 in condition A
    • Item 1 in condition B
    • Item 1 in condition C
    • Item 1 in condition D
    • Item 2 in condition A
    • ...
    • Item 2 in condition D
    • ...
    • Item 32 in condition A
    • ...
    • Item 32 in condition D.
  2. Insert a header row naming all the variables in your file. In the lab, we follow a naming scheme like VariableName, which is easy to read and avoid spaces, special symbols, etc. that would lead to problems later when we import results into the script running the experiment or the analysis software (even later).

  3. Create a column called Item. Enter the item numbers. E.g. for the example above, it would be 1,1,1,1,2,2,2,2,3,...,32,32,32,32 in that column.

  4. Create a column called List.

  5. Copy the following formulate into the List field in the first data row (second row in the sheet overall): =MOD(ROW()-1+k+ReferenceToItemCellInSameRow,k)+1, where ReferenceToItemCellInSameRow refers to the cell with the item number in the same row and k is the number of conditions of your experiment. So, for example, if List in column A and Item is in column B, you would enter the following formula into the List cell in row 2: =MOD(ROW()-1+4+B2,4)+1, which will assign this stimulus to list 1.

  6. Now copy the cell to the remaining cells in the List column. This should assign lists in the desired way. For example, for the example above with 4 conditions and 32 items, you should see the following numbers in the List column:

    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
    • 1
    • 3
    • 4
    • 1
    • 2
    • 4
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
    • 4
    • ...
  7. Check!

Now we almost have the four lists, but we still need to fill in fillers and we need to determine the order of items. Usually, no two items should follow each other immediately.

Making the final lists: Adding fillers and determining the order of stimuli

All lists have the same fillers. Recall that fillers have no conditions. So, yes, all lists actually have the very same filler stimuli. It's a good idea to have at least one filler between two items in the list. Since we usually have at least twice as many fillers as items that is an easy thing to construct. What should be avoided, however, is that fillers and items are distributed according to some pattern, e.g. filler, filler, item, filler, filler, item, filler, filler, item, ... That is not good, since subjects may pick up on such patterns.

So, here's a good way to create lists where each item and each filler occurs in the same position across lists:

  1. Create an excel sheet that contains all stimuli for List1 and has an extra column for a random number (use the random number creation function of excel).
  2. Sort the fillers and items a couple of times according to the random number column, until it seems that there is a nice pseudo-random order to things, e.g. filler, item, filler, filler, item, filler, item, filler, filler, filler, item, filler, filler, item, etc. That's good. If not mentioned otherwise, there are a couple of important constraints that should be considered:

    • Avoid two adjacent items. That is, always have at least one filler intervene between two items
    • Avoid patterns. For example, if the experiment has four conditions avoid orders where the items would be going through conditions a,b,c,d,a,b,c,d,a,b,c,d,.... Instead, we want orders that order conditions in an unpredictable way, e.g. a,b,c,d,b,a,d,c,a,d,c,b,...
    • As in the example pattern in the previous line, we prefer orders where the closest items is not in the same condition as the current item.
  3. Create a new column called Trial and enter numbers from 1 ... n in ascending order. This now encodes in which order the stimuli of List1 will be presented. (If you use formula to create these numbers: be careful when you resort of copy the numbers!)
  4. Sort the list back into the order it had in step 1 above.
  5. Create an excel sheet for each of the remaining lists, as you have done for List1 in step 1 above.
  6. Copy the Trial column from the List1 sheet into all the other List sheets.

Fillers

Fillers are often just considered stimuli that aren't of further interest to the experimenter. It is dangerous though to underestimate the importance of fillers. There are many examples where fillers matter a lot. Fillers may determine what participants take to be the task. Consider, for example, the difference between having only grammatical or also ungrammatical fillers in some task paradigms.

Generally, fillers have two main functions:

Several consequences follow from these two central purposes of fillers:

What the final spreadsheet is like

Now let's start by downloading some example stimuli from a real experiment that was run in Professor Jaeger's lab: ExampleLists.xlsx
It's in the current Excel format (XLSX), although we can also handle the older (XLS) format. Excerpted below are the first four rows of the first sheet from the example:

Experiment

ItemName

Condition

Sentence1

Question1

Answer1

filler

97

-

The librarian chose books that the library patrons would enjoy.

Did the librarian pick out the books?

Y

filler

72

-

The prodigy finished the puzzle in just a few hours.

Did the prodigy complete the puzzle?

Y

controlL1

1

AMV

The excited fans moved through the crowd and sauntered to front stage.

Was the place empty?

N

filler

60

-

The storekeepers were afraid that riots would ensue after the hometeam won the championship.

Were some people worried after the hometeam won?

Y

Entering Items

What the applet will do

MoinMoin Appliance - Powered by TurnKey Linux