Creating Stimuli For Web-base Self Paced Reading

Terminology

Latin square design

The idea behind a Latin square design (a between-subject design) is to have each participant see each item exactly once AND to see all conditions equally often AND across lists each item should be seen equally often in all its conditions. Since each participants sees exactly one list of the experiment, this means that each list should contain each item only once (in one of its condition) and all conditions equally often. One thing that follows from this is that the number of items that an experiment should have should be a multiple of the number of the experiment's conditions. For power consideration, we often have between 3- to 6-times as many items as there are conditions in the experiment (and an equal number of subjects, but that's another matter). For more subtle effects, you may need even more items.

Items and lists

How should the items and conditions be distributed across lists? Let's consider a 2 x 2 design, with the four conditions a1, b1, a2, and b2. The minimum number of lists we will need is four. Let's say that we use 8-times more items than condition (a.k.a. 8 items per condition) and that List1 looks like this (prior to sorting and prior to the inclusion of fillers, more on that later):

So, List1 contains each item only once, and each condition (a1, b1, a2, b2) occurs equally often (8 times). Now we want a second list that fulfills these constraints and brings us closer to the third constraints stated above, that --across lists-- each item should be seen equally often in all its conditions. To achieve this, we construct List2 by simply shifting the condition one up. So the condition of Item1 in List2 will be the one of Item2 in List1, etc. The last item of List2 will occur in the same condition as the first item of List1

If we repeat this for List3 and List4 (see below), you will see that --across lists-- each item occurs exactly once in each of its conditions, and --within lists-- each item occurs once and all conditions occur equally often across items. That's what we want. (Latin square designs are most powerful, if each item is seen equally often in each condition across all participants. That is, we want each list to be seen by equally many participants, but that is a matter to be kept track of later when we run the experiment)

etc.

Fillers

All lists have the same fillers. Lists typically have at least one filler between two items in the list. Since we usually have at least twice as many fillers as items that is easy to satisfy. What should be avoided, however, is that fillers and items are distributed according to some pattern, e.g. filler, filler, item, filler, filler, item, filler, filler, item, ... That is not good, since participants may pick up on such patterns. One way to avoid this is to fully randomize lists, however, that might result in situations where participants see a long chain of items (and even worse if they are all the same condition). An alternative is to use a pseudo-random order where the items and fillers are intermixed somewhat randomly but avoiding excessively long chains of similar trials. For the purposes of this experiment we will pseudo-randomize stimuli using pre-written scripts so don't worry about this, but be aware that this means that you cannot specify your items or fillers to appear in a particular order.

Fillers are often just considered stimuli that aren't of further interest to the experimenter. It is dangerous though to underestimate the importance of fillers. There are many examples where fillers matter a lot. Fillers may determine what participants take to be the task. Consider, for example, the difference between having only grammatical or also ungrammatical fillers in some task paradigms.

Generally, fillers have two main functions:

Several consequences follow from these two central purposes of fillers:

What your sheet should look like

Creating lists for latin square design with a good mix of fillers between items can be done automatically. However, the format given to those scripts needs to follow a very specific format. Here is an example from a real experiment run in Professor Jaeger's lab on what your sheet should look like: ExampleLists.xlsx
It's in the current Excel format (XLSX), although we can also handle the older (XLS) format, and the comma separated value (CSV) format. Excerpted below are the first four rows of the first sheet from the example:

Experiment

ItemName

Condition

Sentence1

Question1

Answer1

control

1

AMV

The excited fans moved through the crowd and sauntered to front stage.

Was the place empty?

N

control

1

ARC

The excited fans moved through the crowd broke apart from Jane accidentally.

Was the place empty?

N

control

1

UMV

The excited fans flew through the crowd and sauntered to front stage.

Was the place empty?

N

control

1

URC

The excited fans that were moved through the crowd broke apart from Jane accidentally.

Was the place empty?

N

control

2

AMV

The aging professors phoned about the midterm and were surprised by the workload.

Was somebody concerned about a homework set?

N

control

2

ARC

The aging professors phoned about the midterm were surprised by all the questions.

Was somebody concerned about a homework set?

N

...

filler

41

-

The parcel arrived too late to be of any use.

Did the package arrive on time?

N

filler

42

-

The espresso machine was broken for months before finally being fixed.

Was the espresso machine fixed?

Y

...

Entering Items

What the web experiment applet will do

MoinMoin Appliance - Powered by TurnKey Linux