[wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/Syllabus Syllabus] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/Assignments Assignments] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/People People] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/CorporaTutorials Corpora & Tutorials] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/References Readings] | [http://lsa2009.berkeley.edu/courses/lsa125.html Offical LSA course page]


Reading and References

We've put together a couple of general readings suggestions for corpus-based research on psycholinguistics in addition to the specific readings mentioned on the syllabus. They are listed below the references.

1. References

AttachList

2. Reading Themes

Each section below summarizes a couple of papers on a particular issue that will be covered in class. We don't at all expect you to read all these papers, it's more to give you pointers for further readings. At the end of each section you find what we identify to be a good entry reading on that topic.

2.1. Accessibility: Availability and Alignment in Sentence Production

Syntactic variation has been attributed to accessibility. For the purpose of this class, accessibility refers to ease of retrieval. Accessibility-based accounts for e.g. word order alternations say that the relative accessibility of the referents described by the different constituents affects speakers' word order preferences.

Two specific proposals have been discussed and tested in detail in the literature. Psycholinguistic alignment accounts (e.g. Bock and Warren, 1985) state that speakers prefer to align conceptually accessible referents with higher grammatical functions (this accounts resemble linguistic accounts of alignments, as e.g. in Aissen, 2003; Bresnan et al., 2001). Availability accounts, on the other hand, state that speakers prefer to mention accessible referents early in the sentence (Levelt & Maassen, 1981; Ferreira, 1996; Ferreira and Dell, 2000). For English these two accounts make very similar predictions, but for other languages they don't necessarily. We recommend Branigan et al. (2007) for a direct comparison and summary of previous work. See also Jaeger and Norcliffe (in press) for a summary of the relevant cross-linguistics work.

2.2. Length and Word Order in Sentence Production

John Hawkins' books from 1994 and 2004 are both classics. We may scan some portions of these, but definitely read Hawkins (2007).

And please read one of the following (whichever ones you don't read are optional):

Yamashita and Change (2001): Gives a Hawkins-style account of ordering preferences in a production experiment with native speakers of Japanese. Choi (1997): This could also go in the Availability and Alignment section, but the paper deals with ordering preferences driven by discourse and other factors in an LFG framework. Arnold et al. (2000): This study tries to tease apart the contributions of syntactic weight and discourse status in post-verbal constituent ordering. Gildea and Temperley (2008): This is an interesting attempt to test the hypothesis that dependency length minimization constrains grammar, insofar as grammars are optimal systems for simultaneously minimizing numerous dependency lengths. The paper is very technical (from a computational linguistics journal), but you can get the gist from the introduction if you don't want to wade through all the details.

2.3. Ambiguity Avoidance in Sentence Production

Please read Haywood et al. (2005) and Arnold et al (2004).

Optional reading: Kraljic and Brennan (2005)

2.4. Uniform Information Density

Uniform Information Density is a recently emerging account of language production (Jaeger, 2006; Levy & Jaeger, 2007; Jaeger, submitted, in prep), according to which speakers' choices in production are driven by a preference to distribute information uniformly across the linguistic signal. Information is defined information theoretically (Shannon, 1948) with reference to probability distribution (the probable an event is the more information its occurrence carries).

Uniform Information Density has been tested against corpus data from phonetic reduction (Jaeger & Kidd, 2008; building on Bell et al., 2003, 2009), morpho-syntactic reduction (Frank & Jaeger, 2008), syntactic reduction (Jaeger, 2006, submitted, in prep; Levy & Jaeger, 2007), and against inter-clausal planning (Gomez Gallo et al., 2008). Data from the distribution of disfluencies and gestures has also been argued to be supporting the principle of Uniform Information Density (Cook et al., 2009).

Short introductions can be found in Levy and Jaeger (2007, rather technical) and Frank and Jaeger (2008). A more in depth discussion in journal format is found in Jaeger (submitted).

2.5. Psycholinguistic Corpus-based work on Syntactic Variation

For some examples, of corpus-based psycholinguistic research on syntactic production, see:

Example of a corpus-based approach using mixed logit models are given in Bresnan et al. (2007) and Jaeger (submitted).

3. Sociolinguistic Corpus-based work on Syntactic Variation

For some examples, of corpus-based sociolinguistic research on syntactic production, see:

Both are very nice papers that are easy to understand.

3.1. Grammaticization and Gradient Grammaticality in Syntactic Variation

Please read Bresnan and Hay (2007) and Torres Calcoullos and Walker (2009)

Optional: Bresnan et al. (2007)

3.2. Statistics for Corpus-based Research

Modern corpus-based research mostly employs multiple regression methods. Since corpus-based work usually involves clustered data (data from different speakers, different groups, etc.) the employed statistical methods need to somehow correct for the resulting violation of the assumption of independence. This can be done, for example, via bootstrapping or by means of multilevel (mixed) models.

Most papers on these models are still hard to read, but there are some pretty readable introductions to ordinary and multilevel regression methods for language researchers.

Baayen (2008) provides a selection of examples, case studies, and some conceptual background, along with tons of R code to run regression and mixed models on language data. You can download the book for free from his website.

Most research on syntactic production requires binomial or multinomial models (because the outcome we're analyzing are categorical). Jaeger (2008) provides an introduction to mixed logit models. Readable applications of such models to corpus data are found in Bresnan et al. (2007) and Jaeger (submitted; see also Jaeger, 2006).

A wonderful introduction to linear mixed models and model comparison over such models is found in Baayen et al. (2008).

For a discussion of statistics with respect specifically to sociolinguistic corpus research, have a look at Johnson (2009).

For further references and advanced tutorials, see [wiki:HlpLab/StatsCourses our HLP lab stats course page] and search the entries of the [http://www.hlplab.wordpress.com/ HLP lab blog]. Also consider subscribing to the R-lang email list, a list specifically designed to help language researchers using R.


[wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/Syllabus Syllabus] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/Assignments Assignments] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/People People] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/CorporaTutorials Corpora & Tutorials] | [wiki:HlpLab/LSA09/References Readings] | [http://lsa2009.berkeley.edu/courses/lsa125.html Offical LSA course page]


MoinMoin Appliance - Powered by TurnKey Linux