Differences between revisions 3 and 4
Revision 3 as of 2012-02-01 14:58:03
Size: 4704
Editor: 10
Comment:
Revision 4 as of 2012-02-01 15:59:55
Size: 4700
Editor: 10
Comment:
Deletions are marked like this. Additions are marked like this.
Line 9: Line 9:
-Infants ~6 months of age can still discriminate phonetic contrasts in non-native languages; lose this skill by 10 months of age in favor of better discrimination of native language phonetic contrasts; this is important for minimal pair discrimination; phonetic categorical modulation seems to occur based on lexical information, perhaps accounting for regional dialect/pronunciation changes ----
-''Does lexical information feed back to pre-lexical selection of phonemes?'' This study tests this theory. Feedback systems for learning distinct from "on-line" feedback systems. ----
Infants ~6 months of age can still discriminate phonetic contrasts in non-native languages; lose this skill by 10 months of age in favor of better discrimination of native language phonetic contrasts; this is important for minimal pair discrimination; phonetic categorical modulation seems to occur based on lexical information, perhaps accounting for regional dialect/pronunciation changes ----
''Does lexical information feed back to pre-lexical selection of phonemes?'' This study tests this theory. Feedback systems for learning distinct from "on-line" feedback systems. ----
Line 12: Line 12:
-Speakers presented with recordings of fluent Dutch speaker for training, 3 groups of subjects. Group 1 heard 20 words ending with ambiguous /f/-/s/ spectrum sounds followed by 20 words with unambiguous /s/ sounds. Group 2 heard 20 words ending with ambiguous /f/-/s/ spectrum sounds followed by 20 words with unambiguous /f/ sounds. 3rd group was control, heard ambiguous /f/-/s/ continuum endings for all words. Groups were given a lexical decision and phoneme categorization task on 3 lists of words similar to the training words, with some filler. '''Results:''' Subjects were faster to label unambiguous-ending words as legal words than words with ambiguities, unsurprisingly. Subjects who heard ambiguous /f/ sounds and unambiguous /s/ sounds were more likely to categorize an ambiguous sound as /f/, and the opposite was true of the second group. This supports the hypothesis of feedback from lexical context cues. However, the authors note that this result could have been the result of selective adaptation to a fricative along the /f/-/s/ continuum, or due to "a contrast between the ambiguous phoneme and the unambiguous endpoint." (p.15) ---- Speakers presented with recordings of fluent Dutch speaker for training, 3 groups of subjects. Group 1 heard 20 words ending with ambiguous /f/-/s/ spectrum sounds followed by 20 words with unambiguous /s/ sounds. Group 2 heard 20 words ending with ambiguous /f/-/s/ spectrum sounds followed by 20 words with unambiguous /f/ sounds. 3rd group was control, heard ambiguous /f/-/s/ continuum endings for all words. Groups were given a lexical decision and phoneme categorization task on 3 lists of words similar to the training words, with some filler. '''Results:''' Subjects were faster to label unambiguous-ending words as legal words than words with ambiguities, unsurprisingly. Subjects who heard ambiguous /f/ sounds and unambiguous /s/ sounds were more likely to categorize an ambiguous sound as /f/, and the opposite was true of the second group. This supports the hypothesis of feedback from lexical context cues. However, the authors note that this result could have been the result of selective adaptation to a fricative along the /f/-/s/ continuum, or due to "a contrast between the ambiguous phoneme and the unambiguous endpoint." (p.15) ----
Line 21: Line 21:
-Listeners attempt to make auditory perception more consistent through a process called normalizing, which makes up for fluctuations in the acoustic signal. They do this by maintaining a few, broadly defined phonemic categories in order to account for variations in speech. Recently, research has shown that people may adjust to speakers by narrowing these phonetic categories based on context and speaker variation, which the text refers to as '''perceptual learning''' or '''perceptual recalibration'''. The article makes references to Norris et al. (2003) in which listeners adjusted their perceptual categories to adjust to the acoustic signal in an ambiguous context, but goes on to suggest that perceptual learning may happen on a speaker, word or even phonemic level. The following experiment is conducted to see the extent of perceptual learning that listeners have when presented with a speaker which produces an odd phoneme, in this case, /d/. ---- Listeners attempt to make auditory perception more consistent through a process called normalizing, which makes up for fluctuations in the acoustic signal. They do this by maintaining a few, broadly defined phonemic categories in order to account for variations in speech. Recently, research has shown that people may adjust to speakers by narrowing these phonetic categories based on context and speaker variation, which the text refers to as '''perceptual learning''' or '''perceptual recalibration'''. The article makes references to Norris et al. (2003) in which listeners adjusted their perceptual categories to adjust to the acoustic signal in an ambiguous context, but goes on to suggest that perceptual learning may happen on a speaker, word or even phonemic level. The following experiment is conducted to see the extent of perceptual learning that listeners have when presented with a speaker which produces an odd phoneme, in this case, /d/. ----

Norris et al. (2002)

Introduction


Infants ~6 months of age can still discriminate phonetic contrasts in non-native languages; lose this skill by 10 months of age in favor of better discrimination of native language phonetic contrasts; this is important for minimal pair discrimination; phonetic categorical modulation seems to occur based on lexical information, perhaps accounting for regional dialect/pronunciation changes


Does lexical information feed back to pre-lexical selection of phonemes? This study tests this theory. Feedback systems for learning distinct from "on-line" feedback systems.


Experiment 1


Speakers presented with recordings of fluent Dutch speaker for training, 3 groups of subjects. Group 1 heard 20 words ending with ambiguous /f/-/s/ spectrum sounds followed by 20 words with unambiguous /s/ sounds. Group 2 heard 20 words ending with ambiguous /f/-/s/ spectrum sounds followed by 20 words with unambiguous /f/ sounds. 3rd group was control, heard ambiguous /f/-/s/ continuum endings for all words. Groups were given a lexical decision and phoneme categorization task on 3 lists of words similar to the training words, with some filler. Results: Subjects were faster to label unambiguous-ending words as legal words than words with ambiguities, unsurprisingly. Subjects who heard ambiguous /f/ sounds and unambiguous /s/ sounds were more likely to categorize an ambiguous sound as /f/, and the opposite was true of the second group. This supports the hypothesis of feedback from lexical context cues. However, the authors note that this result could have been the result of selective adaptation to a fricative along the /f/-/s/ continuum, or due to "a contrast between the ambiguous phoneme and the unambiguous endpoint." (p.15)


Experiment 2


Kraljic et al. (2006)

Introduction


Listeners attempt to make auditory perception more consistent through a process called normalizing, which makes up for fluctuations in the acoustic signal. They do this by maintaining a few, broadly defined phonemic categories in order to account for variations in speech. Recently, research has shown that people may adjust to speakers by narrowing these phonetic categories based on context and speaker variation, which the text refers to as perceptual learning or perceptual recalibration. The article makes references to Norris et al. (2003) in which listeners adjusted their perceptual categories to adjust to the acoustic signal in an ambiguous context, but goes on to suggest that perceptual learning may happen on a speaker, word or even phonemic level. The following experiment is conducted to see the extent of perceptual learning that listeners have when presented with a speaker which produces an odd phoneme, in this case, /d/.


Experiment 1


The experiment run was similar to Norris et al. (2003) in that listeners heard a series of words with ambiguous /d/-/t/ sounds randomly inserted in a list of unambiguous words (in place of an unambiguous /d/ sound, such as croco?ile and to?al [total]) with the hypothesis that if perceptual learning were to occur, listeners would be more likely to classify these sounds as /d/ during a lexical decision task, due to lexical knowledge and adaptation to the speaker's accent. Listeners were also required to place /b/-/p/ sounds on a continuum, with the hypothesis that if listeners shift their perceptual categories in a more general way, we would observe the shift on other continua, but if it was done on a more specific basis, we would not see this effect. Results: Listeners performed very well on the lexical decision task, and like in the Norris et al. (2003) study, listeners who were tested with more ambiguous /d/-/t/ phonemes in a word which should have a /d/ categorized ambiguous phonemes as a /d/ or a /b/ more than the subject group exposed to more /d/-/t/ ambiguous phonemes in place of a /t/. Control groups did not show this effect. EDIT: This study was run with multiple speakers, which is significant because listeners shifted their learning between voices.


Discussion


This study gives evidence to the theory that listeners adjust perceptual categories on a speaker- and phonemic- basis, even for stop consonants, in a domain-general way, as evidenced by the effect on the /b/-/p/ continuum as well. In addition, listeners are able to apply perceptual learning to other speakers, which is useful for understanding inconsistencies in speech like accents.

ThomasDavison (last edited 2012-02-29 05:44:43 by 10)

MoinMoin Appliance - Powered by TurnKey Linux